Published in Skeptic Volume 20 number 2
By Felipe Nogueira
Interview with Hart: cover story on Skeptic |
Carl Hart is an associate
professor in the departments of Psychology and Psychiatry at Columbia
University, and a research scientist in the Division of Substance Abuse at the
New York State Psychiatric Institute. He is a member of the National Advisory
Council on Drug Abuse and on the board of directors of the College on Problems
of Drug Dependence and the Drug Policy Alliance. After receiving his B.S. in
psychology at the University of Maryland and his Ph.D. in experimental
psychology and neuroscience at the University of Wyoming, he went on to publish
many papers in prestigious scientific journals for
which he was made a Fellow by the American Psychological Association. In 2012 he co-authored with Charles Ksir the highly regarded textbook Drugs,
Society, and Human Behavior (McGraw-Hill).
As a neuropsychopharmacologist
Dr. Hart researches the effects of drugs on human physiology and behavior. For
example, one of his experimental objectives was to understand how crack cocaine
users would respond when given a choice between the drug and another appealing
option—money. Contrary to what most people would think, Hart’s experiment revealed
that drugs users can behave rationally, choosing other appealing options than destructive
drugs. Thus, the medical model of addiction that treats it like a disease is
not fully accurate when compared with, say, AIDS or cancer, which patients
cannot simply choose not to have. Many people do quit drug use, even addicts.
The question is, why?
Being a distinguished
drug scientist is only part of Hart’s fascinating personal history with drugs.
He was raised in a poor neighborhood in Miami where in high school he not only
used drugs but also sold marijuana, and in the process committed petty crimes.
To make matters worse, most of his relatives didn’t recognize the value of
a formal education, so he ended up joining the United States Air Force, which
gave him a deeper appreciation of the importance of knowledge, especially
scientific knowledge and the ability to think critically, eventually leading
him to become the first African-American tenured science professor at Columbia
University.
In 2013, he
published High Price: A Neuroscientist’s Journey of Self-Discovery that Challenges
Everything You Know About Drugs and Society (Harper), a book about research,
myths, laws and public policy about drugs written for a general audience. The
book is also a memoir, where Hart discusses his personal life and how he
turned out to be the scientist he is now. High Price received
the 2014 PEN E.O. Wilson Literary Science Writing Award, and he has since
appeared as a regular guest on talk shows such as Bill Maher’s Real Time on
HBO, where on the September 27, 2013 show the host made his usual plea for the
legalization of marijuana, to which Dr. Hart added that cocaine, heroine, and
methamphetamines should also be decriminalized so that people can get the help
they need and we can empty out our prisons of such victimless crimes. He schooled
Bill O’Reilly on the facts about addiction (texting doesn’t count), pointed out
to him that the last three presidents of the United States smoked pot in their
youth, and that, in fact, marijuana use among high school kids has declined
from 37% in 1978 to 22% today (O’Reilly insisted it was higher, to which Dr.
Hart responded bluntly “you’re wrong”).
In this sense
Carl Hart is the mythbuster of drugs, the debunker of addiction myths, and a
true skeptic of the pseudoscience and nonsense underlying this country’s
attitudes about drugs and drug addicts.
Skeptic: Many people believe marijuana is a gateway drug. Is it?
Hart: It all depends what is meant by “gateway drug”. What
people often mean is that marijuana leads to hard drugs. That is not true. It is true that the vast majority of people
who use heroin and cocaine, for example, used marijuana before they used these hard
drugs. But, then you look at the fact that the vast majority of marijuana
smokers don’t go on to use those drugs. So, it’s not a gateway drug. It’s
illogical to make that sort of statement. It would be like saying that “the last
three presidents of United States used marijuana before they became president.
Therefore, marijuana is a gateway drug to the white-house.”
Skeptic: Those who use crack cocaine
didn’t use alcohol or tobacco before using marijuana?
Hart: Most used alcohol and tobacco before they used crack
cocaine; of course, they also drank water or ate a fruit. Crack cocaine users
engaged more in petty crimes than those who use marijuana, so we can also say
petty crimes is a gateway to harder drugs. You can think of any number of
behaviors associated with people who go on to use heroin and call those
behaviors gateway, but that’s not a proper scientific conclusion to draw.
Skeptic: Is there
a difference between crack and cocaine?
Hart: Pharmacologically,
there is no difference. The major thing the people are seeking when they use
cocaine is the cocaine base. Powder cocaine contains not only the base, but a
hydrochloride salt that makes it stable and decreases the likelihood to smoke
it. If you want to smoke it, you have to remove the hydrochloride portion,
which does not contribute to biological effects of cocaine. When people are
describing the difference between crack cocaine and powder cocaine, what they
are really describing is the difference between the ways the drugs are taken.
The onset of effects is faster when you smoke it than when you snort it. But
you can dissolve powder cocaine in water and inject it in your veins and have
the same effect as you do from smoking the crack.
Skeptic: Since it
hits the brain faster, is crack more addictive than cocaine?
Hart: Smoking
crack cocaine and injecting powder cocaine dissolved in water hits the brain at
the same speed. Nonetheless, this is one of the arguments that some have made.
But there is no drug that we punish as a result of the route of administration
besides crack cocaine and powder cocaine. And we make that distinction in the United
States because crack cocaine was associated with poor black people. When we
look to powder cocaine in United States in the early 1980s, there was no drug
that produced more violence than powder cocaine, but no one passed new laws, in
part, because the people engaged were not black people; they were primarily
white people. So as a society we are not being honest about this.
Skeptic: People
think one dose is enough to get them addicted. Can you explain why this is not
true?
Hart: By
definition, drug addiction is behavior that disrupts your psychosocial
functions, your job, your family life, and the behavioral disruptions have to
occur on multiple occasions. So, if you use a drug once and you have a
disruption once, by definition that is not an addiction. Addiction requires
work. There is no one hit of anything
that causes anyone to get addicted. People make these statements based on pure
hyperbole attempting to get an emotional rise out of the listener.
Skeptic: How
addictive is tobacco compared to other drugs?
Hart: At 1 in
3, or 33% of people who smoke will become addicted to tobacco, that is the drug
that we say causes the most addiction. By comparison, 15% of those who use alcohol
will become addicted. The rate for marijuana is about 10%. Heroin is about 25%,
and cocaine (and crack cocaine) ranges from 15% to 20%.
Skeptic: Psychopharmacologist
David Nutt, author of Drugs Without the
Hot Air, published a study in 2010 that called alcohol the most harmful
drug. Do you agree with this?
Hart: I am a
fan of David’s work and of his advocacy to educate the public, and I admire
what he does. I think he tries to make the public understand that when we think
about whether a drug is toxic, we have to look to multiple measures. And in one
measure alcohol may be seen as the most toxic drug. For example, when we think
about withdrawal from a drug, when people abruptly discontinue long-term use of
a drug alcohol is clearly the most dangerous because you can actually die from
alcohol withdrawal. You can’t die from withdrawal from heroine, crack cocaine,
tobacco, or marijuana. When trying to determine the potential toxicity of a
drug, it all depends on what measure you’re looking at.
Skeptic: I’ve
heard a lot of comments about how addictive people behave. For example, some
people say crack users are “zombies”. I’m not sure what it means to be a drug
zombie, but what is it people are thinking when they use such expressions to
describe people under the influence of a drug? That is, how do addicted people
behave?
Hart: I’m a
psychologist who studies human behavior. But all of us behave, and we most of
us think we’re experts in understanding human behavior because of this fact.
But most people are not experts and that’s part of the problem with such subjective
and anecdotal reports. People see somebody misbehaving in some way and they
know that person uses a drug. Therefore, they conclude that the misbehavior is
caused by that substance. No! You can’t draw that conclusion. There are
multiple factors at work, and these multiple factors need to be teased apart.
You have to consider the person’s psychological history, whether or not other
drugs were on board, whether the person was sleep deprived, whether the person
interacted with someone who angered them, etc. People often fail to consider
these other factors. That’s why anecdotes alone are insufficient to provide an
adequate explanation of behavior.
Skeptic: In one of
your experiments you gave participants two choices, money or drugs. What did
you find and what does it mean?
Hart: Of the
things people say about drugs users, particular about the drug addicted, is that
they only respond to their drug of choice: if you give them an opportunity to
use their drug, they’re going to take it over anything else. It was a simple
experiment in which we had escalating amounts of money that we offered subjects.
We found that when you increase the monetary value, you decrease their choice
to take the drug—they choose money instead; they behaved rationally.
Skeptic: Why do
people get addicted?
Hart: It’s a
difficult question and one that science should be really focused on. Recently
we’ve been concentrating on finding biological mechanisms of addiction. But
frankly we’ve not found any that are compelling so we must look to other things
as well, such as co-occurring psychiatric illnesses like depression, anxiety, and
schizophrenia, which increase the likelihood of being addicted. Addiction is a condition
primarily characterized by not tempering your behavior when it comes to certain
drugs. But there are people who do not temper their behavior in a wide variety
of domains, not just with drugs, because they just haven’t learned the skills
to do that. So, that increases the likelihood of becoming addicted if they indulge
in drug-taking behavior because taking a drug requires a person to be
responsible, for example when driving an automobile. You can’t be irresponsible
driving automobile; you might hurt yourself or someone else. If you haven’t
learned those responsibility skills, it increases the likelihood of you
becoming addicted. If you don’t have many alternatives in life that are better
than drugs, it increases the likelihood of you becoming addicted. All these
factors are critically important when we’re trying to determine why someone
became addicted compared to someone else who did not.
Skeptic: You
mentioned biological mechanisms for addiction. There is the dopamine hypothesis,
but you mentioned in your book that there are problems with it. Can you
clarify?
Hart: Let’s
talk about the simplistic version of the dopamine hypothesis first. Drugs like
cocaine and amphetamines increase dopamine, which in turn increases pleasure.
So researchers said drug users are trying to increase their dopamine and so
dopamine was considered to be a critical factor. This was an important theory
because it helped us to organize several types of experiments, but it came
about in 1960s when we had identified only five or six neurotransmitters.
Today, we know about more than 100 neurotransmitter substances and yet the
theory hasn’t been appreciably updated. So, we’ve learned a lot about the
complexity of the brain in terms of how neurotransmitters interact with each
other. Rather than one neurotransmitter being released at one time, there are
multiple neurotransmitters that are released together in order to produce
effects. And, sometimes, they are co-localized in the same cells. The old
dopamine theory does not take into account this new knowledge. For me, the
dopamine hypothesis is too simple to explain complicated behavior. Plus, we
have some observable information that can help people right now: Does the
person have a co-occurring disorder? Is the person responsible? We can
manipulate many variables to help people now; you can’t assist drug users
immediately with the dopamine hypothesis, and it doesn’t help anybody in
treatment; it doesn’t help in any practical level. It’s on the level that
science is trying to figure it out and we’re not close, if we’re talking about
dopamine.
Skeptic: What
about pharmacological treatments for drug addiction, such as cocaine?
Hart: A large part of my career has been spent trying to
develop medications to help people with cocaine addiction, but we have been
unsuccessful in finding a medication that works. It seems the best medication
for cocaine addiction is cocaine itself. In Switzerland heroin is used to treat
heroin addiction, and successfully so. When we think about treatment there are
a variety of components that are necessary, not only the drug: psychosocial
support, therapy to figure it out what’s going on with the person’s addiction,
do they have employment, do they have a social network? All of these factors
are incorporated into the heroin treatment program in Switzerland, and it’s
been successful. You can do a similar thing with cocaine, but you have to have
all these other auxiliary components in place.
Skeptic: What are the harms
caused by amphetamines?
Hart: The first lesson people should really know is that there are potential
harms for all the drugs that we’ve talked about it. However, when you only
emphasize the potential harms, people who use drugs stop listening to you,
because they know there are other effects (that’s why they use them). The major
concern about amphetamines is that they have a powerful effect on the
cardiovascular system; it increases blood pressure and heart rate. This is not
good for people who have a compromised cardiovascular system; they should not take
large doses of amphetamines. Amphetamines disrupt sleep, and sleep deprivation
can cause several physical and psychological problems, even without any drugs
use. Amphetamines also disrupt food intake, which is critically important for
the proper functioning of the body. Those are the concerns that people who take
amphetamines should be aware of, rather than other concerns that are often
emphasized in the media and even by scientists. About cognitive decline,
amphetamines are approved to enhance cognitive functioning in order to treat
attention deficit disorder. So, the notion that amphetamines are causing
cognitive dysfunction is simply inconsistent with the history of behavioral
science we have with these drugs.
Skeptic: Another idea you hear a lot is that marijuana
or cocaine kills nerve cells. Is that any evidence of that?
Hart: Any psychoactive drugs in large doses can kill brain cells. However, these
doses are so large that they the drugs would be so unpleasant to humans that they
wouldn’t take those doses again after experiencing their effects, if they
survived it. These doses are 20-80 times of what people usually takes. In
addition, there is no evidence that long-term use of doses within the range that
humans take produces neurotoxicity. Neurotoxicity certainly can happen, but the
likelihood that it happens to people using drugs within the range that humans take
is very low.
Skeptic: Why do you advocate for decriminalization rather than legalization of
drugs?
Hart: Of all these drugs that we’ve talked about, nicotine in tobacco is probably the one that kills you
with the smallest amount of it. 50 mg of nicotine would kill half of us. In
contrast, with 50 mg of cocaine or heroin, you just feel really nice. So, we
have to think why nicotine is legal even though so little of it is necessary to
kill people. It’s legal because we have societal structures, we have education
about nicotine, and we know how to keep people safe with it. One cigarette
contains 1 mg of nicotine and one pack contains 20 cigarettes. It requires people
to smoke multiple packs immediately to kill themselves. We’ve packaged nicotine
in a way that substantially decreases the likelihood of someone getting immediately
harmed. We have to figure out how to package other drugs and educate people in
a way to keep them safe. We have not done that yet, because we don’t have the
social structures in place. I see decriminalization, in part, as the
intermediary step to regulation, like we regulate alcohol and tobacco. But we
have to put those structures in place to keep people safe, because all of these
drugs are potentially dangerous, although all of them can be used safely.
Skeptic: We can
compare different places (countries, states, cities) with different drugs laws
(prohibited, decriminalized, legalized) regarding several outcomes, such as
rates of drug use, drug-related deaths, drug-related homicides, and crime. What
can we say about these kinds of measurements? That is, what are the effects of
decriminalization and legalization on such outcomes?
Hart:
I think a comparison of Portugal and the U.S. would be
instructive here. In 2001, Portugal decriminalized all drugs including heroin
and cocaine. Overall, they have increased spending on prevention
and treatment, and decreased spending for criminal prosecution and
imprisonment. The number of drug-induced
deaths has dropped, as have overall
rates of drug use, especially among young people (15-24 years old). In general,
drug use rates in Portugal are similar, or slightly better, than other European
Union countries, and they are doing better than us in the U.S. In other words,
Portugal’s decriminalization has been successful. No, it didn’t stop all
illegal drug use. That would have been an unrealistic expectation. Portuguese
continue to get high, just like their contemporaries and all human societies
before them. But they don’t seem to have the problem of stigmatizing,
marginalizing, and incarcerating substantial proportions of their citizens for
minor drug violations. Together, these are some of the reasons that I think
decriminalization, or even legalization should be considered as potential
options in the United States.
Skeptic: If you
were the director of drug control polices in United States, what would be your
short- and long-term policies?
Hart: I would
first work to decriminalize all drugs so that people no longer run the risk of
being incarcerated for drug possessions. Too many of our citizens are
languishing in prison unnecessarily for drug violations. I would also work to
change the way we educate about drugs such that it would no longer be
acceptable to exaggerate the harms of drugs as a scare tactic. Education would
first function to keep our citizens safe. In the long-term, I’d work to
regulate all drugs so that we decrease the likelihood of drug users obtaining
and using drugs that are adulterated with chemicals more dangerous than the drugs
themselves. My major goal as director would be keeping people safe, with the
understanding that some people will use drugs as humans have always done.
Skeptic:
What was your goal when you wrote the book High Price?
Hart: The goal was to
communicate to the general public. The first goal of the scientific community
is to keep their lab funded and, sometimes, this is inconsistent with larger
goals of society. The research on drug abuse is mainly focused on the bad
effects of drugs, because the major funder is National Institute of Drug Abuse,
which funds 90% of world’s research in this area. National Institute of Drug
Abuse goal is focused on the bad things that
happen
with drugs, and scientists are shaped by this goal. Society at large has the
goal of being safe, and scientists have the goal of “don’t
use these drugs”, so it’s inconsistent. I wanted to make the public understand
that their goal and the scientist’s goal are different, that their goal and the
goal of the police man who is talking to them are different, and that their
goal is also different from the goal of media person who writes stories about
drugs.
Skeptic: At Skeptic we advocate for evidence-based instead
of faith-based thinking, as do you. What do you think is the best approach to shift
people from faith-based to evidence-based thinking?
Hart: When you
try to change people’s view about something that they think they know, in some
cases they feel threatened and insulted because they worked really hard to
acquire this knowledge. Now you tell them they’re wrong, and that’s hard for most
of us to accept. So, the approach has to be one that acknowledges the work they
have put in to acquire their knowledge, even if their knowledge is outdated or
inaccurate. On the one hand, you say “you know, I was just like you, I got
fooled. They fooled me, like they just fooled you. Here it’s how I see it
differently now, and how you can see it differently too.” You have to make some
connection with the audience, with the people, in order to move them along.
Sometimes we all get impatient because we don’t have the time to make the
connection, so we say “the evidence shows this and you’re wrong.” That usually
doesn’t work. I understand why people become impatient because they hope others
will just follow the evidence like they do, but it doesn’t work that way on a
regular enough basis.
Skeptic: What do
you think is critical to being a good scientist?
Hart: Always
to try to disprove your own hypothesis. If you’re not doing that, you increase
the likelihood of following dogma. So the critical thing is to design
experiments that can disprove your own hypothesis.
Skeptic: Thank
you for this thoughtful interview.